Chapter 61 Vision, reading and dyslexia
Reading
There are diverse causes of poor reading; some are environmental, others have a biological basis. Dyslexia is the most common cause of poor reading. However, abnormalities in the visual and auditory systems can cause a reading problem, and therefore need excluding.
Reading requires the extraction of meaning from print. This is complex, but for most people it is effortless. However, learning to read poses a challenge, drawing on a range of language and cognitive skills. In the early stages, children have to learn to convert letter strings to sounds or phonemes. For example, the word “cat” has three phonemes: /k/ /a/ /t/. The ability to draw meaning from these sounds is called semantics; once word meanings are identified, grammatical skills integrate meanings within sentences and beyond.
The orthography conventions of a language consist of the rules and regularities that comprise the writing system. As reading development proceeds, children abstract the mappings between the symbols of the orthography (graphemes) and the sounds of words (phonemes); in turn these sounds make contact with word meanings (semantics).
In an alphabetic language (such as English), poor phonological skills compromise the development of grapheme-phoneme mappings. This is the cornerstone of the current understanding of the term dyslexia.1,2
Dyslexia
A definition of the term dyslexia is emerging. This is due to be published in DSM-5 (Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, American Psychiatric Association), and is outlined in Box 61.1.3
Box 61.1 Proposed definition of dyslexia
DSM-5
A. Difficulties in accuracy or fluency of reading that are not consistent with the person’s chronological age, educational opportunities, or intellectual abilities
Multiple sources of information are to be used to assess reading, one of which must be an individually administered, culturally appropriate, and psychometrically sound standardized measure of reading and reading-related abilities
B. The disturbance in criterion A, without accommodations, significantly interferes with academic achievement or activities of daily living that require these reading skills
The major change (from the 1994 definition) is the removal of discrepancy between general cognitive ability and reading skills. While a discrepancy between achievement (in reading) and intellectual ability may still be present, it is now appreciated that all dyslexic children, irrespective of intellectual ability, have the same poor phonological skills.2 Furthermore, all groups demonstrate similar improvement as a result of evidence-based phonological interventions.4,5
An independent report, authored by Sir Jim Rose, formerly Her Majesty’s Inspector and Director of Inspection for the Office for Standards in Education (Ofsted), defined dyslexia as outlined in Box 61.2.6 Point 2 illustrates the three areas of phonological processing initially described by Wagner and Torgeson:7
a. Phonological awareness: the ability to attend to and manipulate sounds in words.
b. Phonological memory: memory for speech-based information – sometimes called verbal memory.
c. Naming (providing the spoken label for visual referent) or verbal processing speed.
Box 61.2 Definition of dyslexia
Rose Report on Dyslexia 2009
1. Dyslexia is a learning difficulty that primarily affects the skills involved in accurate and fluent word reading and spelling
2. Characteristic features of dyslexia are difficulties in phonological awareness, verbal memory, and verbal processing speed
3. Dyslexia occurs across the range of intellectual abilities
4. It is best thought of as a continuum, not a distinct category, and there are no clear cut-off points
5. Co-occurring difficulties may be seen in aspects of language, motor co-ordination, mental calculation, concentration, and personal organization, but these are not, by themselves, markers of dyslexia
6. A good indication of the severity and persistence of dyslexic difficulties can be gained by examining how the individual responds or has responded to well-founded intervention
Most dyslexic children demonstrate weakness in all three areas. Most educational psychologists agree that one standard deviation from the mean for that age signifies a significant weakness.
Dyslexia is common (5% of the population).8 It is distributed equally between the sexes,9,10 is familial, and persistent.
Dyslexia was originally a behavioral description (inaccurate or dysfluent word reading or spelling), but is now based on a defect at a cognitive level (poor phonological processing).8 Functional MRI findings demonstrate (in adults) activity in the left parietotemporal region during word analysis involving phonemes, and the left occipital temporal region for automatic rapid responding. The latter predominates in skilled reading.8 There is evidence that children with dyslexia rely on other areas (such as the inferior frontal gyrus and other right hemisphere sites) during reading, possibly as a result of compensatory processes. Anterior cortical areas, involved in articulation, may contribute in developing phoneme awareness (forming the words with the lips and tongue).
Reading assessment
Phonological processes are examined in a variety of ways (Table 61.1). Most of these tests have been validated in children of different ages.
Dyslexia management
Prospective studies show that 8-year-old dyslexics demonstrated weak letter knowledge in reception classes at school (age 4−5 years) and poor phoneme awareness in year 1.11,12 It is desirable to identify these children early and to supply early support before these children fail.4,13
Evidence-based reading instruction is administered, based on the three areas of phonemic awareness, phonics, and reading fluency. This is combined with vocabulary and comprehension strategies. Phonemic awareness concentrates on manipulating phonemes with letters, focusing on one or two manipulations rather than multiple types, teaching in small groups, and specific instructions about counting and manipulating the sounds. These strategies work better in younger children, emphasizing the importance of early detection and implementation. Fluency is helped most by guided oral reading, which impacts on fluency, improving comprehension. Large amounts of private reading, with little feedback, are less helpful.
There are many publications (see Snowling4) demonstrating reading improvement using phonological based packages. Delay in applying these can lead to harm. Children who fail become disruptive, avoid reading, and cannot access academic material.
The visual system
A working visual system is required to see the script. The areas available to examine and manipulate are up-stream from the pathology of dyslexia, as outlined above. The eye care practitioner’s role is to exclude any visual problem. Elucidating whether any visual abnormality coexists and whether it is causative or secondary to poor reading may be difficult (see Handler14 and the AAO joint statement15).
School screening generally examines distance vision. In the context of poor reading, the examiner must concentrate on near tasks (without excluding distance visual acuity).
The clinical work-up
History
The clinician asks about visual symptoms, reading development, and general development. Visual symptoms can be common (depending on how the questions are asked) and can include blurring, movement of the text, and headaches. Developmental delay, cerebral palsy, and prematurity can be associated with poor accommodation and visual perception problems.

Stay updated, free articles. Join our Telegram channel

Full access? Get Clinical Tree

