Monofixation Syndrome



Monofixation Syndrome


Marshall M. Parks



As the evaluation of strabismus therapy became increasingly critical, attention was focused on a relatively large group of patients who had a small residual deviation. This group attracted particular attention because, in addition to the consistent findings of a deviation measuring 8Δ or less and good fusional vergence amplitudes, there was a scotoma within the deviated eye that prevented diplopia.

Further interest was stimulated when it became apparent that some patients with small deviations had no history of strabismus. Anisometropia was identified early as a frequently associated factor in the nonstrabismic patients. Even more interesting was the discovery that some of the population free of strabismus and anisometropia had this same disorder. After it was noted that the common denominator in all patients with a small deviation was a small facultative macular scotoma within the visual field of one eye, binocular perimetry studies on a control series of patients with straight eyes revealed that some of these patients also had a macular scotoma. It was also recognized that the rare patient with a unilateral macular lesion, having straight eyes and extramacular fusion, has the organic counterpart to the unilateral functional facultative macular scotoma just described.

Consequently, a group of patients, either with or without a small deviation, from varied sources constitutes a specific ophthalmologic entity characterized by monofixation, due to the macular scotoma precluding bifixation and active extramacular binocular vision. The entity is referred to as the monofixation syndrome.

Initial interest in the monofixation syndrome came by way of the small angle deviations. Patients with these small angle deviations were referred to as “flicker cases,” by the British, because the cover test revealed a small “flick” as the deviated eye assumed fixation. This syndrome soon became confused with the normal fixation disparity described by Ogle1 and others. In addition, this condition has been called fixational disparity and also referred to as subnormal binocular vision due to the lack of central fusion. In 1956 Jampolsky2 described how some of these patients have greater alternate cover measurements than cover-uncover measurements, and he emphasized that this is diagnostic of the disorder. He described the suppression within the central retinal area in one eye of these patients, and he used this as an explanation for solving the diplopia caused by the minimal deviation. Jampolsky further reasoned that the peripheral portion of Panum’s fusional space is sufficiently large to permit fusion with normal retinal correspondence (NRC). His opinion regarding the lack of success with orthoptic treatment for these patients to convert them to centrally fusing rather than suppressing the central retinal area is clearly stated. Jampolsky and co-workers3 also noted the paucity of small angle exodeviations as compared to the frequent number of cases of convergent small angle deviations. In 1962 Jampolsky4 referred to the monofixation syndrome as “fusion disparity.” He implied that there is normal fusion, except for the absence of bifoveal fixation. He chose the term “fusion disparity” to separate a monofixation syndrome from fixation disparity, which is a normal physiologic entity. There are two obvious dissimilarities between fixation disparity and the monofixation syndrome (Jampolsky’s fusion disparity). The quantity of deviation does not exceed 10 to 14 minutes of arc in fixation disparity, but it may be as large as 8Δ in the monofixation syndrome. In fixation disparity, both macular areas function simultaneously; whereas, in the monofixation syndrome, one or the other macula does not function during binocular vision.

The impossibility of accurately naming this condition in accordance with the established semantic code in common usage for ocular motility and binocular vision was apparent early after the initial interest developed in this large group of patients. Appraised according to one respect, the patient was heterotropic, but in another respect, hetero-phoric. Any term selected to identify these patients was arbitrary. In 1961 the name “monofixational phoria” was applied to those patients with a deviation that was greater by alternate cover than by cover-uncover; it was claimed that the deviation was made partially latent by extramacular fusion while the image projected onto the deviated eye’s macula was not seen.5 At that time, interest was directed only to the small angle aspect of the deviations, and physicians were unaware that many patients without a deviation also had the identical sensory finding of a scotoma of one macula. Jampolsky’s concept of NRC peripheral fusion acquired by the normal stretched out peripheral Panum’s space was accepted, and the NRC seemed to be confirmed by the findings from binocular perimetry performed during dissociated conditions. The following significant facts about the monofixation syndrome also added by this report were:



  • Anisometropia, in addition to strabismus, was established as a cause.


  • In some patients, neither strabismus nor anisometropia was present, and these patients were defined as having primary monofixational phoria; those with strabismus and anisometropia were defined as having secondary monofixational phoria.


  • Stereoacuity was first related to the nature of the fixation present: poor in monofixation and good in bifixation.


  • The facultative absolute scotoma was revealed by binocular perimetry.

In 1966 Lang criticized monofixational phoria as a name for small angle strabismus since there is a manifest tropia. Burian’s definition of heterophoria is a “deviation of the eyes kept latent by fusion; heterotropia is a patent (manifest) deviation of the eyes in the absence of fusion.”6 Lang7 proposed that the syndrome be known by the full name of microtropia unilateralis anomalo fusionalis, but he suggested that it be referred to ordinarily as microtropia or microstrabismus. In 1967 Helveston and von Noorden8 used the term “microtropia” to describe an inferred small angle strabismus in their amblyopia patients with eccentric fixation whose amblyopic eye did not make a movement to assume fixation and who grossly appeared to have straight eyes. The majority of their patients were anisometropic. Since, by visuscopy, the fixation point was adjacent to the macular borders, they inferred that the strabismus angle was ultra small. Others can confirm these findings in many patients with the monofixation syndrome whose poor sighted eye either has not responded to amblyopia therapy or has never been treated; the syndrome occurs either as a primary condition or secondary to strabismus, anisometropia (or the two combined), or a macular lesion. These patients seem to have monofixational orthophoria since there is no detectable shift in either eye by the cover test. Perhaps Helveston and yon Noorden are correct in their assumption that in some cases there probably would be a discernible shift were it not for the slight eccentric fixation in the amblyopic eye; therefore, the patient is not orthophoric. However, use of the term “microtropia” is not justified to describe the patients without shift to cover-uncover when Lang previously used the term to describe patients having a deviation by cover-uncover. The group described by Helveston and von Noorden probably represents only one of many various groups of patients within the overall monofixation syndrome.

The semantic structure that evolved as a result of many attempts to label various categories of patients that constitute the monofixation syndrome has become a monstrosity. Surely such terms as retinal slip, fixation disparity, esophoria with fixation disparity, fixational disparity, flicker cases, subnormal binocular vision, convergent fixation disparity, pathologic fixation disparity, monofixational phoria, fusion disparity, strabismus spurius, microtropia unilateralis anomalo fusionalis, microtropia, and microstrabismus will vanish from ordinary usage.

There are three principal reasons for the past difficulties encountered in naming this syndrome: (1) an element of both phoria and tropia is present and whichever feature the author chooses to emphasize determines the selection; (2) fixation disparity, as a name for a specific physiologic process in binocular single vision, was plagiarized since the condition under discussion seemed to be a pathologic extension of the same process; and (3) the names selected revealed the lack of a total concept of the syndrome. As the syndrome was gradually put together, the lack of organization in naming each of the facets is now apparent.

Essentially, the patients with this syndrome have straight or almost straight eyes and a form of binocular vision in which their inability to bifixate is proved by a demonstrable scotoma in the visual field of the nonfixating eye during binocular vision. This essential monofixating feature and other associated features are always present, while others may be either present or absent. Fusional vergence amplitudes are always associated with the monofixation syndrome. The variable features associated with this syndrome are a history of strabismus, anisometropia, a unilateral macular lesion, amblyopia, eccentric fixation, orthophoria, phoria, small tropia, and possibly a larger deviation by alternate cover than by cover-uncover. The majority of the patients with the monofixation syndrome have gross stereopsis; occasionally, the only exception is a patient with congenital esotropia who is straight and has sensory and motor fusion. The name that fits all of these features is simply “the monofixation syndrome.”


ETIOLOGY

Since patients with the monofixation syndrome have associated strabismus, anisometropia, a unilateral macular lesion, or an inherent inability to fuse similar images on each macula, it is helpful to consider each of these conditions as a separate etiologic factor. The monofixation syndrome is caused by any of the preceding four factors or by any combination of them.

Among patients with the monofixation syndrome caused by strabismus, there is a significantly greater frequency of corrected esotropia than of corrected exotropia. According to one author’s experience,9 approximately 66% of the successfully treated horizontal strabismic patients are esotropic and 34% are exotropic; yet of the strabismic patients who develop the monofixation syndrome after treatment, approximately 90% are esotropic and 10% are exotropic. Obviously, the chances that an exotropic patient will develop the monofixation syndrome are much less than the chances that an esotropic will develop the syndrome.

The probable explanation for the greater frequency of the monofixation syndrome in patients with corrected esotropia than in those with corrected exotropia is the difference between the constancy and intermittency of the deviation prior to treatment. The patient with constant tropia loses the bifixation habit completely. The possibility of restoring it after deviation is eliminated appears not to be directly related to the patient’s age at the time bifixation is lost and to the duration of the constant deviation. Since esotropic patients prior to receiving therapy tend to have constant deviations with greater frequency than exotropic patients, one should anticipate that a greater percentage of those who are esotropic will remain monofixating patients after treatment. In a study made by the author at the time the deviation was brought under control, 70% of the esotropic patients and only 21% of the exotropic patients were constantly tropic.

It is tempting to conjecture that amblyopia is more prevalent in esotropic than in exotropic patients since in the study just mentioned, 40% of the esotropic patients were amblyopic and only 3% of the exotropic persons were amblyopic out of 100 consecutive patients having horizontal strabismus. Yet among patients with the monofixation syndrome, 78% of the esotropic and 57% of the exotropic are amblyopic. The latter fact suggests that the incidence of the monofixation syndrome as a final treatment status is increased in both esotropic and exotropic patients if amblyopia exists. However, it does not follow that amblyopia is the cause of the monofixation syndrome after the deviation has been eliminated. Evidence to the contrary was found in the fact that 24% of the patients with the monofixation syndrome were never amblyopic, and an additional 16% still had the monofixation syndrome after their amblyopia had permanently been cured by occlusion therapy. A more plausible concept is that both amblyopia and the monofixation syndrome result from the same cause, but the development of amblyopia requires one additional factor. Both are produced by prolonged and constant strabismic deviation in the infant or young child; but, in addition, development of amblyopia requires the constant exclusion of one eye from fixating rather than alternate fixation. Hence, not all patients with the monofixation syndrome following strabismus therapy have amblyopia.

Congenital esotropic patients appear to have a different reason for monofixating, despite the fact that peripheral fusion was acquired by early surgical elimination of the deviation; they seem to have an inherent inability to bifixate. Most congenital esotropic patients obtain extramacular fusion if the eyes have been straightened by surgery when they were less than two years of age, but they never obtained bifixation. Most congenitally esotropic patients who obtain extramacular fusion after their eyes have been straightened do not develop stereopsis. Although this result occurred in 61% of patients we studied, this combination of extramacular fusion and no stereopsis has been observed only on the surgically straightened congenital esotropic patients. Of the straightened strabismic patients studied, all others with fusion manifested stereopsis capability. It is tempting to speculate why bifixation never develops and why stereopsis often does not develop in these patients even though the congenital esotropia is surgically corrected by 6 months of age. Perhaps there is some justification for Worth’s suggestion that these children have a deficit in the fusion faculty. Proof that extramacular fusion is attained by early surgical intervention in a high percentage of congenital esotropic patients partially discredits this concept. However, there may be some merit in Worth’s thesis since a defect in the faculty serving macular binocular vision remains a distinct possibility.

Regardless of how this observation is explained, none of the therapeutic regimens offered the infant with congenital esotropia to date has produced bifixation.

Anisometropia is another etiologic factor that presents an additional obstacle to macular fusion. A clear image on one macula and a blurred image on the other offers little reward for the effort involved in integrating the two into a unified perception. Presuming that similarly clear macular images are required during infancy for establishment of bifixation, one realizes that discovery of anisometropia at an older age is too late to expect bifixation to result from prescription of optic correction for equally clear images on each macula. Unless strabismus is also present, it is difficult to discover the anisometropia during infancy. The question that naturally follows is the age at which anisometropia must be optically treated to permit bifixation to develop. Too few facts are available to answer this question.

Only gold members can continue reading. Log In or Register to continue

Stay updated, free articles. Join our Telegram channel

Jul 10, 2016 | Posted by in OPHTHALMOLOGY | Comments Off on Monofixation Syndrome

Full access? Get Clinical Tree

Get Clinical Tree app for offline access