Temporal Properties of Vision




(1)
University of Sydney, Sydney, Australia

 




Overview




1.

The visual system in a changing environment



  • The visual system responds to variation in light over time, allowing instantaneous interpretation of a rapidly changing environment.


  • It concentrates on useful information (e.g., contrasting boundaries of objects, temporal changes in location and magnitude) and discards irrelevant features.


  • Successive retinal images are stored, integrated, differentiated, and erased, resulting in the perception of apparently stable scenes.


  • Temporal responsiveness varies between scotopic and photopic conditions.

 

2.

Temporal resolution of stimuli



  • The visual system is only able to detect stimuli at finite time intervals.


  • Stimuli presented closer together than this finite time are treated as a single stimulus event.


  • The time at which two discrete stimuli are just detected is the temporal threshold or limit of temporal resolution.

 


Temporal Summation and the Critical Duration (Tc)


The duration of a light stimulus influences its:

(a)

Ease of visibility

 

(b)

Subjective appearance

3.

Temporal summation



  • Temporal summation describes the influence of stimulus duration on its visibility [1].


  • It occurs because a longer duration stimulus emits more photons over time than a brief stimulus of the same intensity. Multiple, sequential photons may be required for the light to be seen.

 

4.

Critical duration (Fig. 22.1)

A347009_1_En_22_Fig1_HTML.gif


Fig. 22.1
Schematic representation of stimulus duration influencing detection threshold: Bloch’s law and critical duration (Tc)




  • The critical duration (Tc) is the maximum time period over which temporal summation can occur.


  • Beyond Tc temporal summation ceases and detection depends on luminance alone.


  • For flashes briefer than Tc, the chance of visual detection of a light source is based on luminance (B) and duration (t), described in Blochs law [2]:


    
$$ Bt=C, $$
    where C is constant

 

5.

Factors that influence the critical duration (Tc)



  • In humans, Tc is approximately 40–400 ms, depending on factors outlined below [1, 3, 4].

    (i)

    Background luminance



    • Tc is greater in dark (scotopic) than bright (photopic) conditions [5, 6].


    • In scotopic conditions, temporal summation enhances sensitivity to low-luminance stimuli; however, in photopic conditions, it interferes with temporal and spatial contrast discrimination [7, 8].

     

    (ii)

    Stimulus size



    • Tc is greater for small stimuli and smaller for large stimuli [9, 10].


    • This applies predominantly to photopic conditions; in scotopic conditions, size has less influence.

     

    (iii)

    Spectral composition



    • Tc is greater for isolated chromatic stimuli than achromatic (mixed wavelength) stimuli [11].


    • For colored lights, it is greater for shorter wavelength hues (blues) than longer (reds) and decreases with increased chromatic saturation [12, 13].

     

    (iv)

    Other factors



    • Tc is greater for complex perceptual tasks and for high acuity tasks [3, 14].


    • It is also influenced by retinal location [12].

     

 

6.

Critical duration and assessment of contrast threshold



  • Thresholds to light detection are measured using flashes longer than Tc, so that flash duration is removed as a variable that may influence threshold.


  • This is important in static perimetric testing: each test stimulus must be present for longer than Tc (see Chap. 23, The Visual Field).

 

 


The Broca-Sulzer Effect (Fig. 22.2)




A347009_1_En_22_Fig2_HTML.gif


Fig. 22.2
The Broca-Sulzer effect





  • Brief stimuli appear subjectively brighter than a longer flash of the same luminance; this is the BrocaSulzer effect [15, 16].


  • As flash duration increases, there is a transient peak brightness at 50100 ms.


  • For stimuli of duration longer than this, subjective brightness is decreased and reaches a plateau of subjective luminance [17].


  • The Broca-Sulzer effect is most apparent for bright flashes and is less significant for dim stimuli.


Troxler’s Phenomenon






  • Troxler’s phenomenon is a time-dependant visual adaptive process (see Chap. 21, Luminance Range for Vision).


  • A fixed retinal image fades from perception in a few seconds; it is restored by a slight movement of the image or the eye [18, 19].


  • It demonstrates the visual system’s reliance on temporal as well as spatial contrast to capture visual information.


  • Troxler’s phenomenon is a neural, not photochemical, phenomenon.


  • The decay is slower with larger, brighter, and more central images [20].


Visual Fixation




1.

Control of fixation



  • During fixation, Troxler’s phenomenon is prevented by repetitive small eye movements [21].


  • These include slow monocular drifts, microsaccades, and tremors [22].

 

2.

Saccadic suppression



  • Saccades are brief voluntary conjugate eye movements to bring an object of regard into central view (see Chap. 18, Neural Control of Eye Movements) [23].


  • During saccades (10–80 ms) visual processing is temporarily suppressed and the visual system is unresponsive to visual input, preventing the sensation of movement and blur [24, 25].


  • Between saccades the eyes make fixed pauses of brief duration (200–300 ms) to take in visual information, during which suppression is released [26].

 


Critical Flicker Frequency




1.

Definition



  • When light is turned on and off repeatedly, it appears to flicker.


  • As the speed of the on/off cycle increases, we eventually perceive the flashes as a single fused light.


  • The critical flicker frequency (CFF) is the transition point of perception from flicker to continuous light.


  • The CFF is a measure of the temporal acuity (resolving power) of the visual system.

 

2.

Factors that influence the critical flicker frequency

(i)

Luminance



  • The FerryPorter law states that CFF increases linearly with log luminance (Fig. 22.3A) [27, 28].

    A347009_1_En_22_Fig3_HTML.gif


    Fig. 22.3
    The Ferry-Porter law, (a) Mixed light (b) Monochromatic light sources


  • The Ferry-Porter law is only valid in photopic states [28, 29].

 

(ii)

Spectral composition



  • When monochromatic light sources are used, the CFF increases linearly with log luminance, according to the Ferry-Porter law.


  • The increase in CFF with luminance is greater for green light and less for red (Fig. 22.3B) [30, 31].


  • This may be due to differences in signal processing speed between green and red cone pathways.


  • The linear increase of CFF with luminance is least for blue light [32].


  • This is because short-wavelength-sensitive cones (and rods) have slower processing speeds than medium-wavelength-sensitive and long-wavelength-sensitive cones.

 

(iii)

Stimulus size



  • CFF increases with stimulus size.


  • This is the GranitHarper law, stating that CFF is linearly proportional to log stimulus area [33, 34].


  • It is only valid in photopic states and with stimuli within 10° of central fixation [3].

 

(iv)

Retinal eccentricity (Fig. 22.4)

A347009_1_En_22_Fig4_HTML.gif


Fig. 22.4
Change in critical flicker frequency with increasing retinal eccentricity (Based on Rovamo and Raninen, 1984) [37]




  • CFF increases with retinal eccentricity within the central 50° of the visual field and then decreases with further eccentricity [3537].

 

(v)

Background luminance/adaptive state



  • In general, CFF increases with greater levels of adaptation.


  • Maximal CFF occurs when background luminance is the time-averaged luminance of the flickering stimulus [3, 38].


  • Photopic flickering lights presented on dark backgrounds results in rod-cone interactions that decrease sensitivity [39].

 

 

3.

The effects of flicker on perception

(i)

Brucke-brightness enhancement effect



  • The apparent brightness of a flickering stimulus varies with the frequency of the flicker, with a maximum apparent brightness at a range 15–20 Hz [40].


  • This is closely related to the Broca-Sulzer effect, as flicker frequency is related to stimulus duration per flicker (Fig. 22.2).

 

(ii)

Talbot-Plateau law



  • The TalbotPlateau law describes the brightness of an intermittent light source with a frequency above the CFF [41, 42].


  • This law states that above CFF, subjectively fused intermittent light and objectively steady light (of equal color and brightness) will have precisely the same luminance.


  • For example, a flickering stimulus at twice the CFF needs to be twice as bright as a steady stimulus.

 

 


Temporal Contrast Sensitivity




Oct 28, 2016 | Posted by in OPHTHALMOLOGY | Comments Off on Temporal Properties of Vision

Full access? Get Clinical Tree

Get Clinical Tree app for offline access