Luminance Range for Vision




(1)
University of Sydney, Sydney, Australia

 




Overview




1.

Mechanisms that broaden our luminance range for vision



  • Our visual system can function over a wide range of light intensities, from starlight to a bright sunny day – a luminance range of 10 10 (10 log units) [1].


  • Several dynamic mechanisms exist that broaden our luminance range for vision in response to a change in ambient level of illumination.


  • They allow the visual system to obtain maximal visual information at each luminance level:

    (a)

    Light-induced changes in pupil size

     

    (b)

    The switch between the scotopic and photopic pathways in our duplex visual system

     

    (c)

    Visual adaptation

     

 

2.

Visual adaptation: change in gain of the visual system



  • Adaptation is an alteration in gain of the visual system.


  • Gain is the ratio of the output signal (neural responses) to input signal (light).


  • Gain increases under dim conditions and decreases under bright conditions.


  • Visual adaptation is largely mediated by:

    (a)

    Retinal photoreceptor mechanisms

     

    (b)

    Retinal neural channel mechanisms

     

    (c)

    Higher center mechanisms

     


  • Each mechanism is involved in light adapation and dark adaptation.

 

3.

Light adaptation



  • Light adaptation is rapid, occuring within seconds [2].


  • As background luminance increases, light adaptation processes maximize spatial, temporal, and chromatic contrast resolution.


  • This allows the visual system to make complex discrimintions such as contour detection, fine spatial resolution, movement, and color perception.


  • However, there is a corresponding decrease in sensitivity. For example, the dark-adapted eye can see stars at night; during daylight (photopic conditions), the stars are equally bright but not seen.

 

4.

Dark adaptation



  • Dark adaptation is the ability of the visual system to recover sensitivity following light exposure.


  • Compared to light adaptation, dark adaptation is a slower process.


  • Recovery is faster in cones, but absolute sensitivity is greater in rods [1, 3, 4].


  • Most dark adaptation occurs within the first few minutes however takes more than 30 min to complete (see Fig. 21.1a) [5].

    A347009_1_En_21_Fig1_HTML.gif


    Fig. 21.1
    (a) The dark adaption curve (based on Hecht et al. [5]). (b) Perceptual reduction in apparent contrast (Adapted from Kohn [6]). Staring at the vertical bars in 1 for 30 s reduces the ability to detect a low-contrast portion (the top) of image 2. The horizontal bars in 3 produce a less strong adaptative effect


  • In very dark conditions, the visual system can detect individual photons, largely mediated by spatial and temporal summation of rod responses to light [79].

 

5.

Contrast adaptation



  • Contrast adaptation affects our visual ability to discern spatial and temporal contrast of stimuli.


  • Unlike light and dark adaptation, it is not influenced by changes in ambient light levels, unless a shift from photopic to scotopic range occurs.


  • The strength of adaptation is related to the similarity between the adapting and test stimuli (see Fig. 21.1b) [10, 11].


  • It occurs in the visual cortex, lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN), and inner retina [6].

 


Mechanisms for Broadening the Dynamic Luminance Range of Vision (Table 21.1)





Table 21.1
Mechanisms for broadening the dynamic luminance range of vision [1, 3, 4, 6, 1054]









































Mechanism

Overview

Sensitivity range (log units)

Time from stimulus to adaptation

Pupil size

Pupil size reduces in bright light and increases in dark, modulating light entering the eye

It is mediated by the pupillary light reflex

1.2

1 s

Switch from scotopic to photopic systems

Scotopic vision facilitates light detection in dim light

Scotopic vision is mediated by rods and their retinal neural channels

Photopic vision facilitates contrast, color, and motion descrimination in medium to bright light

Photopic vision is mediated by cones and their retinal neural channels

Each system can operate over 4–5 log units with some overlap (1–2 log units)

Milliseconds

Visual adaptation

A. Photoreceptor mechanisms

Rod responses are easily saturated by increased ambient light intensity. The range of scotopic sensitivity is greatly enhanced by post-receptoral neural channels

Cones escape saturation no matter how intense the steady light

Light-induced changes responsible for adaptation include:

 1. Pigment bleaching and regeneration

 2. Alterations in intracellular Ca2+ levels

 3. Alterations in phosphodiesterase activity

Rods: 1–2

Cones: 5+

Light adaptation:

 1. Cones: milliseconds

 2. Rods: slower than cones, <1 s

Dark adaptation:

Rate limited by pigment regeneration

 1. Cones: 3–5 min

 2. Rods: 10–30+ min

B. Retinal neural mechanisms

Light and contrast adaptation processes occur in retinal neural channels

Mechanisms include:

 1. Electrical coupling

 2. Lateral inhibition

 3. Ganglion cell adaptation to signal

3

Milliseconds – minutes

C. Higher visual center mechanisms

Higher visual center neurons are capable of contrast but not light adaptation

Contrast adaptation has been demonstrated in the lateral geniculate nucleus magnocellular layers and cortical areas V1, V2, and MT/V5

The strength of adaptation is related to the similarity between the adapting stimulus and test stimulus

Mechanisms include:

 1. Hyperpolarization of the cell-soma membrane

 2. Presynaptic depletion of glutamate

 3. Modulation of neural responses by activity of neighboring neurons

N/A

Milliseconds – minutes



1.

Change in pupil size



  • The pupil size enlarges in the dark to 8 mm and constricts in light conditions to 2.5 mm.


  • This range of pupil diameter sizes allows a 16× change in area for light entry into the eye [13].


  • This corresponds to 1.2 log units of luminance range.


  • Pupillary responses to light are rapid and transient, with a latency of 200–500 ms [14, 55].


  • Although a relatively small contribution to the dynamic luminance range, changes in pupil size provide rapid dynamic shift in light or dark while other adaptive processes are taking place.

 

2.

The duplex system: switching from scotopic to photopic states



  • The retina has a duplex photoreceptor system: the rod (scotopic) and cone (photopic) systems [56].


  • Each system includes photoreceptors and their retinal neural processing channels.


  • Scotopic and photopic vision vary in fundamental ways (Table 21.2) [4852, 5764].


    Table 21.2
    The scotopic vs. photopic systems [4852, 5764]































































     
    Scotopic

    Photopic

    Photoreceptor type

    Rod

    Cone

    Background luminance

    Low

    Medium – high

    Luminance range (log units)

    −4 to −1

    1–4

    Maximum spectral sensitivitya

    507 nm

    555 nm

    Color vision

    Absent

    Present

    Spatial resolution

    Poor

    Good

    Spatial summation

    Increased

    Decreased

    Increment luminance sensitivity

    High

    Low

    Contrast sensitivity

    Low

    High

    Site of maximal acuity

    7° from fovea

    Fovea

    Foveal scotoma

    Present

    Absent

    Temporal resolution

    Poor

    Good

    Critical duration (Tc)b

    Long

    Short


    aThe shift in peak spectral sensitivity between scotopic and photopic conditions is called the Purkinje shift

    bSee Chap. 22, Temporal Properties of Vision


  • The rod system allows maximal light detection sensitivity in scotopic conditions, with high gain at the expense of temporal and spatial acuity.


  • The cone system provides maximal temporal and spatial acuity in photopic conditions with low gain at the expense of sensitivity.


  • In modern urban life, the majority of our vision uses the photopic system; only in exceptionally dark conditions (e.g., starlight, dark rooms) do we rely on the scotopic system [3].


  • As background light intensity shifts from low to high luminance levels, so does our reliance from the rod to cone systems.


  • Mesopic conditions are intermediate between scotopic and photopic, such as a moonlight night; vision in these conditions is mediated by interaction between the rod and cone systems [15].

 

3.

Photoreceptor mechanisms of visual adaptation

(See “Photoadaptation in rods and cones” in Chap. 8, The Retina)



  • The magnitude and speed of photoreceptor membrane potential responses to light stimuli are influenced by background luminance levels [21].


  • The range of responses is much less for rods than cones [19, 20].


  • Significant post-receptoral changes extend the scotopic system’s range beyond that of rods [18].


  • Mechanisms include:

    (i)

    Visual pigment bleaching and regeneration

     


  • Photoreceptor pigment is rapidly bleached on bright light exposure, resulting in separation of the chromophore from opsin (see “The phototransduction cascade” in Chaps. 8, The Retina, and 9, The Retinal Pigment Epithelium).


  • Free opsin activates transducin directly, although less powerfully than metarhodopsin II.


  • This decreases cytoplasmic Ca2+, reducing the amplitude of the transduction cascade [1, 4, 23].


  • The decay of photopigment in bright light reduces the magnitude of the photoreceptor response to light, resulting in light adaptation.


  • The dark pigment is slowly regenerated in cones (5–10 min) and rods (30+ min) [1, 5, 24].


  • This results in less free opsin, increased photoreceptor pigment available for light detection and increased light sensitivity.

    (ii)

    Lightinduced reductions in cytoplasmic Ca 2+ levels

     


  • This can occur through several mechanisms independent of pigment bleaching [2529].


  • It causes modulation of the cationic nucelotide-gated (CNG) channels reducing light sensitivity.

    (iii)

    Increased photodiesterase activity in steady light

     


  • This results in more rapid turnover of cGMP, reducing light sensitivity [22].

 

4.

Neural adaptation



  • Neural adaptation mechanisms include retinal and higher visual pathway processes.


  • They provide 1000× (3 log units) of adaptative range.


  • These processes are very rapid (occuring in milliseconds).


  • In the light, they decrease spatial and temporal summation (causing less efficient light detection) and increase surround inhibitory effects (providing more efficient contrast discrimination).


  • Neural adaptation results in light adaptation as well as contrast adaptation [6, 37].

 

5.

Retinal neural adaptation mechanisms

(i)

Electrical coupling of photoreceptor, horizontal, bipolar, and amacrine cells [12].



  • Electrical rodrod coupling is important in dark adaptation.


  • It spatially averages rod signals over large distances, which

    (a)

    Decreases noise filtering at the rod-bipolar junction

     

    (b)

    Increases rod synaptic saturation

     


  • This improves light detection at the expense of image resolution [12].


  • Rodcone coupling encourages shift to the photopic range enhancing light adaptation [17]. Additionally, it allows maximal rod responses to light to reduce cone sensitivity [16].


  • Dopamine release by some amacrine cells enhances light adaptation by reducing coupling of inner retinal neurons [53, 54] (see “Inner retinal circuitry” in Chap. 8, The Retina).

 

(ii)

Lateral inhibition



  • Lateral inhibition by horizontal and amacrine cells enhances center surround antagonistic receptive fields [30, 31].


  • This enhances borders and contrast at the expense of spatial summation and light sensitivity.


  • It is important in light and contrast adaptation.

 

(iii)

Ganglion cell adaptation processes



  • Several presynpatic and cell-soma mechanisms exist that modulate ganglion cell responses.

    (See “Inner retinal circuitry” in Chap. 8, The Retina) [3235]

 

 

6.

Adaptation in higher visual areas

Oct 28, 2016 | Posted by in OPHTHALMOLOGY | Comments Off on Luminance Range for Vision

Full access? Get Clinical Tree

Get Clinical Tree app for offline access